kids encyclopedia robot

Stevens's power law facts for kids

Kids Encyclopedia Facts

Stevens' power law helps us understand how our senses work. It's a scientific idea in psychophysics, which is the study of how physical things (like light or sound) connect to what we feel and sense. This law explains that when a physical stimulus, like a bright light or a loud sound, gets stronger, the feeling or sensation we have from it also gets stronger in a predictable way.

This idea is named after a scientist named Stanley Smith Stevens (1906–1973). Even though some people had similar ideas before, Stevens brought this "power law" back into focus. He published a lot of data in 1957 to show that it was true for many different senses.

The basic idea of the law can be shown with a math formula:

\psi(I) = k I ^a,

Don't worry, it's simpler than it looks!

  • I stands for the strength of the physical stimulus. This could be how bright a light is, how heavy something feels, or how loud a sound is.
  • ψ(I) (pronounced "psi of I") stands for how strong the sensation feels to you.
  • a is a special number called an "exponent." This number changes depending on what sense we are talking about. For example, the a for how loud something sounds is different from the a for how bright something looks.
  • k is just a constant number that helps the math work out, depending on the units used.

This law is often seen as an improvement over an older idea called the Weber–Fechner law. That's because Stevens' power law can describe how we sense things even when the stimulus is very weak, almost zero.

The table below shows some of the "exponent" numbers (a) that Stevens found for different senses:

Sense Exponent (a) What was being sensed
Loudness 0.67 How loud a 3000 Hz sound was
Vibration 0.95 How strong a 60 Hz vibration felt on a finger
Vibration 0.6 How strong a 250 Hz vibration felt on a finger
Brightness 0.33 A small light in the dark
Brightness 0.5 A single point of light
Brightness 0.5 A quick flash of light
Brightness 1 A single point of light flashed quickly
Lightness 1.2 How light or dark gray papers looked
Visual length 1 The length of a projected line
Visual area 0.7 The size of a projected square
Redness (saturation) 1.7 How red a red-gray mix looked
Taste 1.3 How sweet Sucrose tasted
Taste 1.4 How salty salt tasted
Taste 0.8 How sweet Saccharin tasted
Smell 0.6 The smell of Heptane
Cold 1 How cold a metal touch on the arm felt
Warmth 1.6 How warm a metal touch on the arm felt
Warmth 1.3 How warm a small area of skin felt from heat
Warmth 0.7 How warm a large area of skin felt from heat
Discomfort, cold 1.7 How uncomfortable whole-body cold felt
Discomfort, warm 0.7 How uncomfortable whole-body warmth felt
Thermal pain 1 Pain from heat on the skin
Tactual roughness 1.5 How rough rubbing emery cloths felt
Tactual hardness 0.8 How hard squeezing rubber felt
Finger span 1.3 The thickness of blocks felt by fingers
Pressure on palm 1.1 How much pressure felt on the skin
Muscle force 1.7 How strong muscle contractions felt
Heaviness 1.45 How heavy lifted weights felt
Viscosity 0.42 How thick stirring silicone fluids felt
Electric shock 3.5 The feeling of current through fingers
Vocal effort 1.1 How much effort felt when speaking
Angular acceleration 1.4 The feeling of 5 seconds of rotation
Duration 1.1 How long white-noise sounds felt

How Scientists Measure Sensations

Stevens used a few main ways to figure out how strong people felt a stimulus.

Magnitude Estimation

Imagine a scientist shows you a light and says, "This light has a brightness of 10." Then they show you another light. If the second light seems twice as bright, you would say "20." If it seems half as bright, you would say "5." This is called magnitude estimation with a standard. You are comparing everything to the first "standard" light.

Sometimes, people just start by picking their own number for the first stimulus. Then they keep using numbers that show how much stronger or weaker the next sensations feel compared to the first. This is called magnitude estimation without a standard.

Magnitude Production

This is the opposite of estimation. A scientist might tell you, "Make a sound that is twice as loud as this first sound." You would then adjust a sound until it felt twice as loud to you.

Cross-Modality Matching

This method is really interesting! It's when you match one type of sensation to another. For example, a scientist might ask you to adjust the brightness of a light until it feels as strong as a certain amount of warmth or pressure. This helps scientists compare how different senses work.

Things People Questioned About the Law

Even though Stevens' power law is widely used, some scientists have raised questions about it.

One main point of discussion is that Stevens often averaged the data from many people. While this gave a good overall picture, it might hide the fact that each person senses things a little differently. What feels "twice as loud" to one person might not feel exactly "twice as loud" to another.

Another question is whether the way we measure sensations truly shows a perfect "ratio scale." This means if something feels twice as strong, is it truly twice as strong in a measurable way? Some studies have shown that people don't always interpret numbers in a perfectly exact way when judging sensations.

Also, the way we perceive things can change depending on the situation. For example, a sound might feel less loud if there's a lot of background noise. This means that our sensations aren't always "absolute" or fixed; they can be affected by what's happening around us.

See also

Kids robot.svg In Spanish: Función potencial de Stevens para niños

kids search engine
Stevens's power law Facts for Kids. Kiddle Encyclopedia.